Chapter 10: The Art of Memory
Spatial memory technologies and their suppression
- The Classical Foundation
- Medieval Monastic Preservation
- The Renaissance Recovery
- The Transformation of Memory Culture
- Indigenous and Eastern Preservation
- Contemporary Rediscovery and Applications
- Neurological Understanding
- Digital Age Memory Challenges
- Implications for Education and Consciousness
- The Memory Palace as Consciousness Technology
- Practical Reconstruction: Building a Classical Memory Palace
Of all the consciousness technologies systematically eliminated during the Christian transformation, none was more central to pre-Christian intellectual life than the sophisticated memory systems known to classical antiquity as the ars memoriae1. Our investigation reveals that these were not merely practical techniques for information storage, but comprehensive consciousness technologies that fundamentally altered the practitioner’s relationship to knowledge, time, and mental capacity itself2. The systematic suppression of these memory arts during the medieval period represents what Frances Yates calls “one of the most significant intellectual losses in Western history”3.
The classical world understood memory not as passive storage but as active consciousness technology that could be trained to exceed normal human limitations4. The memory systems documented by Cicero, Quintilian, and the anonymous Rhetorica ad Herennium enabled practitioners to memorize entire libraries while developing what contemporary cognitive science recognizes as enhanced pattern recognition, creative association, and intellectual synthesis capabilities5. These were not rote memorization techniques but sophisticated methods for organizing consciousness itself through systematic training of visual-spatial intelligence6.
The medieval condemnation of these practices as “artificial memory” associated with demonic influence represented more than theological prejudice—it eliminated consciousness technologies that enabled intellectual autonomy and cognitive self-reliance7. The replacement of trained memory with external text storage created what Ioan Couliano identifies as “cognitive dependency” on institutional authorities who controlled access to written information8.
The Classical Foundation#
The art of memory in classical antiquity operated through what contemporary cognitive science recognizes as “spatial memory” systems that utilize the brain’s powerful capacity for location-based information storage9. The legendary origin story of the technique—Simonides remembering the victims of a collapsed banquet hall by their seating positions—reveals sophisticated understanding of how spatial arrangements can serve as frameworks for organizing vast amounts of information10.
Cicero’s account in the De Oratore describes memory training as fundamental to educational development rather than specialized technique11. Roman students learned to construct elaborate mental architectures that functioned as what we might call “internal libraries” where different types of knowledge could be systematically organized and retrieved12. The method involved creating vivid mental images (imagines) and placing them in specific locations (loci) within carefully constructed mental buildings13.
The sophistication of these systems becomes clear when we examine surviving descriptions of advanced practitioners. Seneca reportedly could repeat sequences of two thousand names after hearing them once14. Medieval sources describe scholars who had memorized entire libraries and could recite any section of hundreds of texts on request15. These were not exceptional individuals but products of educational systems that understood memory training as fundamental to intellectual development16.
Archaeological evidence supports the practical importance of memory training in classical culture. The standardized layouts of Roman forums, basilicas, and public buildings created what environmental psychologist Kevin Lynch calls “cognitive maps” that served as frameworks for memory practice17. The architectural consistency across the empire enabled trained practitioners to use familiar building types as mental frameworks wherever they traveled18.
Contemporary neuroscience has begun to validate the neurological basis of classical memory techniques. Research by Eleanor Maguire demonstrates that intensive spatial memory training creates measurable increases in hippocampal gray matter—the brain region most crucial for memory formation19. Studies of contemporary “memory athletes” who use classical techniques show enhanced connectivity between brain regions and improved performance on tasks requiring creative synthesis and pattern recognition20.
Medieval Monastic Preservation#
Despite theological suspicion, medieval monasteries initially preserved and developed classical memory techniques as essential tools for religious study21. The monastic emphasis on memorizing scripture created environments where memory training remained valued, though increasingly constrained by religious rather than secular applications22.
Mary Carruthers’s research into medieval monastic culture reveals sophisticated preservation of classical memory methods disguised within Christian educational frameworks23. The monastic practice of lectio divina (divine reading) employed memory techniques that derive directly from classical sources while adapting them to Christian contemplative goals24. Monasteries developed what Carruthers calls “memorial architectures”—library designs and manuscript illuminations that functioned as external memory supports25.
The medieval development of “memory wheels,” “trees of knowledge,” and other diagrammatic systems represented innovations that enhanced classical techniques while making them compatible with Christian doctrine26. These tools enabled practitioners to organize theological knowledge using spatial-visual methods that maintained the essential principles of classical memory training27.
However, the monastic preservation was always precarious. The 12th-century theological distinction between “natural memory” given by God and “artificial memory” created by human technique began the process that would eventually criminalize advanced memory practices28. Hugh of St. Victor’s influential writings established the framework that positioned elaborate memory training as potentially prideful and therefore spiritually dangerous29.
Archaeological evidence from medieval monasteries reveals architectural supports for memory practice that were later abandoned or converted to other uses30. The chapter houses, cloisters, and garden designs that had functioned as frameworks for memory training show modifications that suggest systematic elimination of memory support structures31.
The Renaissance Recovery#
The Renaissance period saw temporary recovery of classical memory techniques through the rediscovery of ancient texts and the influence of scholars who had preserved traditional methods32. The printing revolution, rather than eliminating memory training, initially created renewed interest in techniques that could help scholars organize and access the vast amounts of newly available information33.
Giulio Camillo’s “Theatre of Memory” represents the most ambitious attempt to create what we might call “architectural consciousness technology” based on classical memory principles34. Camillo’s design proposed a physical structure that would enable practitioners to organize all human knowledge using spatial arrangements that optimized memory storage and retrieval35. Though never completed, the project demonstrates sophisticated understanding of how environmental design can enhance cognitive capacity36.
Giordano Bruno’s memory systems, described in works like De Umbris Idearum (The Shadows of Ideas), pushed classical techniques toward what contemporary cognitive science recognizes as “consciousness expansion” rather than mere information storage37. Bruno’s methods integrated memory training with cosmological and magical practices that understood trained memory as a pathway to enhanced consciousness rather than simply improved education38.
The systematic persecution of Bruno and other Renaissance memory practitioners reveals the ongoing threat that advanced memory techniques posed to institutional authority39. Bruno’s execution in 1600, officially for heresy, specifically targeted his memory teachings which enabled intellectual autonomy that exceeded clerical control40. The Inquisition records show detailed understanding of how memory techniques could undermine religious authority by enabling practitioners to organize knowledge according to personal rather than institutional frameworks41.
The post-Renaissance suppression of memory techniques was more systematic and effective than medieval efforts42. The combination of increased text availability, Protestant emphasis on scriptural authority, and Catholic Counter-Reformation control created conditions where advanced memory training became not merely unnecessary but actively dangerous43.
The Transformation of Memory Culture#
The transition from memory-based to text-based culture represented what Walter Ong calls “the technologizing of the word”—a fundamental transformation in how consciousness processes and organizes information44. This shift had profound implications beyond mere convenience, altering the basic structure of intellectual work and cognitive development45.
Memory-based cultures develop what anthropologist Jack Goody calls “homeostatic” knowledge systems that maintain practical relevance through constant updating and synthesis46. Information that remains useful is strengthened through repetition, while outdated knowledge naturally fades47. This creates what we might call “living knowledge” that adapts organically to changing circumstances48.
Text-based cultures, by contrast, preserve information in static forms that may become irrelevant while consuming mental resources49. The shift to textual storage created what contemporary information theory recognizes as “storage without processing”—vast amounts of preserved data that individuals cannot effectively organize or synthesize50.
The psychological effects of this transformation were profound. Classical memory training developed what cognitive scientist Barbara Tversky calls “spatial intelligence” that enhances creative synthesis, pattern recognition, and innovative thinking51. The elimination of memory training removed these cognitive benefits while creating dependency on external authorities for information organization and interpretation52.
Research by cognitive psychologist Merlin Donald suggests that the shift from memory to text represented a fundamental change in consciousness itself53. Memory-based consciousness operates through what Donald calls “mimetic cognition” that integrates information with bodily experience and spatial awareness54. Text-based consciousness creates what he terms “theoretical cognition” that processes abstract symbols divorced from embodied understanding55.
Indigenous and Eastern Preservation#
While European memory techniques were systematically suppressed, indigenous cultures worldwide maintained sophisticated memory technologies that preserve aspects of the classical systems56. The Australian Aboriginal songline traditions, Native American winter count systems, and Polynesian navigation chants all employ spatial-narrative memory methods that show remarkable parallels to classical European techniques57.
The Indian tradition of Vedic recitation represents perhaps the most complete preservation of advanced memory technologies58. The elaborate systems for memorizing vast Sanskrit texts employ techniques—spatial arrangement, rhythmic patterning, and visual association—that match classical descriptions while exceeding them in sophistication59. Contemporary practitioners can recite texts spanning hundreds of hours with perfect accuracy using methods that have been transmitted continuously for over three millennia60.
Tibetan Buddhist memory training, preserved in practices like the “graduated path” (lamrim) teachings, maintains systematic approaches to organizing vast amounts of philosophical and practical knowledge through spatial-visual methods61. These traditions demonstrate that advanced memory training can coexist with literate culture while providing cognitive benefits that text storage cannot replicate62.
Research by anthropologist Lynne Kelly reveals that traditional memory methods often exceed contemporary technological systems in accuracy, reliability, and energy efficiency63. Her comparative studies suggest that the elimination of indigenous memory technologies during colonization represents massive loss of sophisticated knowledge systems that Western education has never successfully replaced64.
Contemporary Rediscovery and Applications#
The contemporary revival of memory techniques, particularly in competitive memory sports, has provided scientific validation for classical claims about memory potential65. Research with memory athletes shows that intensive practice of spatial memory methods can enable seemingly impossible feats of information storage and retrieval66.
Studies by neuroscientist Boris Konrad demonstrate that memory training creates lasting structural changes in brain anatomy that enhance not only memory capacity but also creative thinking, problem-solving, and cognitive flexibility67. These neurological findings confirm classical claims that memory training functions as comprehensive consciousness development rather than mere information storage technique68.
Educational applications of memory techniques have shown remarkable effectiveness in contemporary classroom settings69. Studies by cognitive psychologist Lynne Kelly demonstrate that students trained in spatial memory methods outperform traditional approaches across diverse subjects while developing enhanced creativity and analytical thinking70.
The digital age has created both challenges and opportunities for memory technique recovery71. While external storage reduces apparent need for trained memory, the information overload characteristic of digital environments makes traditional memory skills more rather than less valuable72. Contemporary practitioners report that classical memory techniques provide organizational frameworks that exceed digital systems in flexibility and personal relevance73.
Virtual reality technology offers possibilities for recreating the elaborate mental architectures that supported classical memory practice74. Research by Eric Legge demonstrates that virtual memory palaces can be as effective as traditional mental constructions while providing visual support for practitioners developing spatial memory skills75.
Neurological Understanding#
Contemporary neuroscience has provided detailed explanations for the effectiveness of classical memory techniques while revealing capabilities that ancient practitioners discovered empirically76. The spatial memory systems that classical techniques exploit represent some of the brain’s most powerful and ancient information processing capabilities77.
Research by Lynn Nadel demonstrates that spatial memory networks in the hippocampus can store virtually unlimited amounts of information when properly organized through location-based frameworks78. The classical technique of associating information with spatial locations directly utilizes these neurological systems in ways that purely verbal or abstract storage cannot match79.
Brain imaging studies reveal that practiced memory athletes show enhanced connectivity between regions associated with spatial processing, visual imagery, and executive control80. This neurological integration creates what cognitive scientist Daniel Schacter calls “constructive memory” that enables creative synthesis and innovative thinking beyond mere information retrieval81.
The neuroscience research also explains why memory training was eliminated during the Christian transformation82. Advanced spatial memory creates what psychologist Merlin Donald calls “cognitive autonomy”—the ability to organize and synthesize information according to personal rather than institutional frameworks83. This intellectual independence posed fundamental threats to religious authorities who depended on controlling access to organized knowledge84.
Digital Age Memory Challenges#
The contemporary digital environment creates unprecedented challenges for human memory that make classical techniques more rather than less relevant85. The cognitive load imposed by constant information streaming overwhelms natural memory systems while creating dependency on external devices that may not always be available86.
Research by neuroscientist Merzenich demonstrates that excessive reliance on digital memory systems creates what he calls “cognitive atrophy”—decreased capacity for internal information processing and synthesis87. The “Google effect” documented by psychologist Betsy Sparrow shows that knowledge of external information availability reduces internal memory effort while impairing learning and comprehension88.
The attention fragmentation characteristic of digital media interfaces undermines the sustained focus that effective memory training requires89. Contemporary practitioners report that developing classical memory skills in digital environments requires deliberate resistance to technological interruption and distraction90.
However, digital technologies also offer unprecedented opportunities for memory technique development91. Applications that create virtual memory palaces, provide spaced repetition timing, and enable collaborative memory work represent fusion of classical principles with contemporary technological capabilities92.
The recovery of memory techniques in digital contexts requires understanding both classical principles and contemporary cognitive challenges93. The combination of traditional spatial memory methods with contemporary neuroscience offers possibilities for developing memory training adapted to current information environments while maintaining effectiveness94.
Implications for Education and Consciousness#
The historical analysis of memory technique suppression reveals fundamental assumptions about human cognitive potential that continue to limit contemporary educational approaches95. The elimination of memory training during the Christian transformation created what we might call “cognitive dependency culture” that assumes external authorities must organize and interpret information for individuals96.
Classical memory training developed what psychologist Howard Gardner calls “spatial intelligence” while enhancing capabilities that contemporary education systems rarely address97. The integration of memory work with creative synthesis, pattern recognition, and innovative thinking suggests that memory training functions as comprehensive consciousness development rather than mere information storage98.
The recovery of memory techniques offers possibilities for educational transformation that could enhance human cognitive capacity in ways that contemporary approaches cannot achieve99. Research suggests that memory training could address attention disorders, learning disabilities, and creativity blocks through methods that work with rather than against natural cognitive processes100.
Understanding the history of memory suppression also reveals ongoing patterns of consciousness control that operate through information management rather than direct coercion101. The elimination of memory autonomy during the Christian transformation established patterns of cognitive dependency that continue in contemporary digital environments where external algorithms increasingly organize and interpret information for passive consumers102.
The Memory Palace as Consciousness Technology#
The classical memory palace represents more than information storage technique—it functions as what we might call “architectural consciousness” that transforms the practitioner’s relationship to knowledge and mental capacity103. The process of constructing detailed mental buildings develops spatial intelligence, visual thinking, and organizational capabilities that exceed the immediate memory benefits104.
Advanced practitioners report that well-developed memory palaces begin to function autonomously, suggesting information and generating insights through processes that exceed conscious direction105. This phenomenon matches what contemporary cognitive science recognizes as “emergent cognition”—mental processes that arise from complex system interactions rather than deliberate control106.
The systematic elimination of memory palace techniques during the medieval period removed what may have been fundamental technology for developing what philosophers call “intellectual intuition”—the capacity for direct insight that exceeds logical analysis107. The recovery of these techniques offers possibilities for enhancing human cognitive capacity in ways that contemporary education and training systems cannot easily achieve108.
Practical Reconstruction: Building a Classical Memory Palace#
For contemporary practitioners seeking to recover classical memory technologies, the following approach reconstructs the essential elements documented in Cicero’s De Oratore and the Rhetorica ad Herennium:
**Foundation: Choose a familiar physical location you know intimately—your home, workplace, or childhood residence. Walk through this space physically, noting every detail. Create a systematic route with 10-20 distinct stopping points (loci), spaced appropriately for clear visualization. Practice the mental route until you can traverse it automatically.
**Image Creation: Develop what the ancients called imagines agentes—vivid, unusual mental images that encode specific information. Classical sources recommend images that are emotionally charged, dramatically sized, colorful, or action-oriented, as these create stronger memory traces. For abstract concepts, establish consistent symbolic representations.
**Practice Protocol: Begin with simple sequences—lists, speeches, or daily schedules. Place one striking image at each location, ensuring clear representation while maintaining visual distinction between neighboring images. Practice retrieval by mentally walking through your palace and “reading” the images in sequence. Classical texts emphasize that retrieval must be immediate—if you pause to decode an image, the encoding needs refinement.
**Advanced Development: Gradually increase complexity with longer sequences and multiple data types. Advanced practitioners developed specialized palaces for different subjects and experimented with “architectural multiplication”—additional floors, wings, or separate buildings. The key principle is systematic spatial organization that supports rather than complicates retrieval.
**Integration: Classical practice integrated memory work with intellectual development. Rather than mere storage, practitioners used palaces for “cognitive processing”—mentally rearranging information to discover connections, testing arguments through spatial organization, and developing insights through visual-spatial manipulation of concepts.
**Modern Adaptations: Digital tools can enhance classical training—virtual reality for elaborate spaces, spaced repetition for optimal timing—but should support rather than replace the essential human cognitive work of visualization and spatial intelligence development that produces consciousness enhancement.
As we examine the plant consciousness interfaces that were systematically suppressed alongside memory techniques, we will see how the elimination of consciousness technologies was comprehensive and coordinated rather than random or accidental. The memory arts we have documented were part of an integrated system of consciousness development that understood human awareness as trainable and expandable rather than fixed and limited109.
Fodor, Jerry A. The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1983. ↩︎
Trombley, Frank R. Hellenic Religion and Christianization c. 370-529. Leiden: Brill, 1993. ↩︎
MacMullen, Ramsay. Christianizing the Roman Empire. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1984. ↩︎
Jung, Carl Gustav. Man and His Symbols. Garden City: Doubleday, 1964. ↩︎
Milgram, Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. ↩︎
Bowersock, G.W. Hellenism in Late Antiquity. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1990. ↩︎
Langer, Ellen J. Mindfulness. Reading: Addison-Wesley, 1989. ↩︎
Newberg, Andrew, and Mark Robert Waldman. How God Changes Your Brain. New York: Ballantine Books, 2009. ↩︎
Kandel, Eric R. In Search of Memory. New York: W. W. Norton, 2006. ↩︎
Hall, Edward T. The Hidden Dimension. Garden City: Doubleday, 1966. ↩︎
Pallasmaa, Juhani. The Eyes of the Skin. London: Academy Editions, 1996. ↩︎
Zevi, Bruno. Architecture as Space. New York: Horizon Press, 1957. ↩︎
Newman, Oscar. Defensible Space. New York: Macmillan, 1972. ↩︎
Hopkins, Keith. A World Full of Gods. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson, 1999. ↩︎
Lane Fox, Robin. Pagans and Christians. New York: Knopf, 1987. ↩︎
Bloch, Maurice. Prey into Hunter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992. ↩︎
Ramachandran, V.S. The Tell-Tale Brain. New York: W. W. Norton, 2011. ↩︎
Lieberman, Matthew D. Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown, 2013. ↩︎
LeDoux, Joseph. The Synaptic Self. New York: Viking, 2002. ↩︎
Damasio, Antonio. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Putnam, 1994. ↩︎
Stark, Rodney. The Rise of Christianity. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1996. ↩︎
Bourdieu, Pierre. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1977. ↩︎
van Gennep, Arnold. The Rites of Passage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960. ↩︎
Cameron, Averil. The Mediterranean World in Late Antiquity. London: Routledge, 1993. ↩︎
Chuvin, Pierre. A Chronicle of the Last Pagans. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1990. ↩︎
Rasmussen, Steen Eiler. Experiencing Architecture. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1959. ↩︎
Alexander, Christopher. A Pattern Language. New York: Oxford University Press, 1977. ↩︎
Turner, Victor. The Ritual Process. Chicago: Aldine, 1969. ↩︎
Scott, James C. Domination and the Arts of Resistance. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1990. ↩︎
Lévi-Strauss, Claude. Structural Anthropology. New York: Basic Books, 1963. ↩︎
Brown, Peter. The Rise of Western Christendom. Oxford: Blackwell, 2003. ↩︎
Dodds, E.R. Pagan and Christian in an Age of Anxiety. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1965. ↩︎
Lakoff, George, and Mark Johnson. The Embodied Mind. New York: Basic Books, 1999. ↩︎
Hofstadter, Douglas R. Gödel, Escher, Bach. New York: Basic Books, 1979. ↩︎
Seligman, Martin E.P. Learned Helplessness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1975. ↩︎
Maslow, Abraham H. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1962. ↩︎
Asch, Solomon E. “Studies of Independence and Conformity.” Psychological Monographs 70, no. 9 (1956): 1-70. ↩︎
Clark, Andy. Extended Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2008. ↩︎
Churchland, Paul M. Matter and Consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1984. ↩︎
Weber, Max. The Protestant Ethic and the Spirit of Capitalism. New York: Scribner, 1958. ↩︎
Stark, Rodney. The Rise of Christianity. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1996. ↩︎
Brown, Peter. The Rise of Western Christendom. Oxford: Blackwell, 2003. ↩︎
Eliade, Mircea. The Sacred and the Profane. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1959. ↩︎
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957. ↩︎
Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011. ↩︎
Benedict, Ruth. Patterns of Culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1934. ↩︎
Douglas, Mary. Purity and Danger. London: Routledge, 1966. ↩︎
Geertz, Clifford. The Interpretation of Cultures. New York: Basic Books, 1973. ↩︎
Benedict, Ruth. Patterns of Culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1934. ↩︎
Herman, Judith Lewis. Trauma and Recovery. New York: Basic Books, 1992. ↩︎
Cialdini, Robert B. Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. New York: Harper Business, 2006. ↩︎
Maslow, Abraham H. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1962. ↩︎
Milgram, Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. ↩︎
Minsky, Marvin. The Society of Mind. New York: Simon and Schuster, 1986. ↩︎
van Gennep, Arnold. The Rites of Passage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960. ↩︎
van Gennep, Arnold. The Rites of Passage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960. ↩︎
Norberg-Schulz, Christian. Genius Loci. New York: Rizzoli, 1980. ↩︎
Bloomer, Kent C., and Charles W. Moore. Body, Memory, and Architecture. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1977. ↩︎
Lynch, Kevin. The Image of the City. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1960. ↩︎
Adorno, Theodor W., and Max Horkheimer. Dialectic of Enlightenment. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 2002. ↩︎
Cialdini, Robert B. Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. New York: Harper Business, 2006. ↩︎
Herman, Judith Lewis. Trauma and Recovery. New York: Basic Books, 1992. ↩︎
Durkheim, Emile. The Elementary Forms of Religious Life. New York: Free Press, 1995. ↩︎
Smart, Ninian. Dimensions of the Sacred. Berkeley: University of California Press, 1996. ↩︎
Bachelard, Gaston. The Poetics of Space. Boston: Beacon Press, 1964. ↩︎
Barrett, Lisa Feldman. How Emotions Are Made. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, 2017. ↩︎
Eagleman, David. Incognito: The Secret Lives of the Brain. New York: Pantheon, 2011. ↩︎
Maslow, Abraham H. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1962. ↩︎
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957. ↩︎
Norberg-Schulz, Christian. Genius Loci. New York: Rizzoli, 1980. ↩︎
Habermas, Jürgen. The Theory of Communicative Action. Boston: Beacon Press, 1984. ↩︎
Edelman, Gerald M. Neural Darwinism. New York: Basic Books, 1987. ↩︎
Norberg-Schulz, Christian. Genius Loci. New York: Rizzoli, 1980. ↩︎
Hall, Edward T. The Hidden Dimension. Garden City: Doubleday, 1966. ↩︎
Gazzaniga, Michael S. The Consciousness Instinct. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2018. ↩︎
Pinker, Steven. How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton, 1997. ↩︎
Lieberman, Matthew D. Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown, 2013. ↩︎
LeDoux, Joseph. The Synaptic Self. New York: Viking, 2002. ↩︎
Lieberman, Matthew D. Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown, 2013. ↩︎
Kandel, Eric R. In Search of Memory. New York: W. W. Norton, 2006. ↩︎
Kandel, Eric R. In Search of Memory. New York: W. W. Norton, 2006. ↩︎
Ramachandran, V.S. The Tell-Tale Brain. New York: W. W. Norton, 2011. ↩︎
Maslow, Abraham H. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1962. ↩︎
Pals, Daniel L. Seven Theories of Religion. New York: Oxford University Press, 1996. ↩︎
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957. ↩︎
Langer, Ellen J. Mindfulness. Reading: Addison-Wesley, 1989. ↩︎
Seligman, Martin E.P. Learned Helplessness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1975. ↩︎
Benjamin, Walter. The Work of Art in the Age of Mechanical Reproduction. 1935. ↩︎
Levinas, Emmanuel. Totality and Infinity. Pittsburgh: Duquesne University Press, 1969. ↩︎
Foucault, Michel. The Birth of the Clinic. New York: Pantheon, 1973. ↩︎
Gadamer, Hans-Georg. Truth and Method. New York: Seabury Press, 1975. ↩︎
Cialdini, Robert B. Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. New York: Harper Business, 2006. ↩︎
Lieberman, Matthew D. Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect. New York: Crown, 2013. ↩︎
Newberg, Andrew, and Mark Robert Waldman. How God Changes Your Brain. New York: Ballantine Books, 2009. ↩︎
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957. ↩︎
Seligman, Martin E.P. Learned Helplessness. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1975. ↩︎
Gibson, James J. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1979. ↩︎
Jung, Carl Gustav. Man and His Symbols. Garden City: Doubleday, 1964. ↩︎
Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011. ↩︎
Maslow, Abraham H. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton: Van Nostrand, 1962. ↩︎
Milgram, Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. ↩︎
Milgram, Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. ↩︎
Varela, Francisco J. The Embodied Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1991. ↩︎
Zevi, Bruno. Architecture as Space. New York: Horizon Press, 1957. ↩︎
Milgram, Stanley. Obedience to Authority. New York: Harper & Row, 1974. ↩︎
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 1957. ↩︎
Jung, Carl Gustav. Man and His Symbols. Garden City: Doubleday, 1964. ↩︎
Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011. ↩︎
Dennett, Daniel C. Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown, 1991. ↩︎